Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 74
Filtrar
1.
Autism Res ; 17(4): 838-851, 2024 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38204321

RESUMO

Gestures are an important social communication skill that infants and toddlers use to convey their thoughts, ideas, and intentions. Research suggests that early gesture use has important downstream impacts on developmental processes, such as language learning. However, autistic children are more likely to have challenges in their gestural development. The current study expands upon previous literature on the differences in gesture use between young autistic and non-autistic toddlers by collecting data using a parent-report questionnaire called the MCDI-Words and Gestures at three time points, 12, 18, and 24 months of age. Results (N = 467) showed that high-likelihood infants who later met diagnostic criteria for ASD (n = 73 HL-ASD) have attenuated gesture growth from 12 to 24 months for both deictic gestures and symbolic gestures when compared to high-likelihood infants who later did not meet criteria for ASD (n = 249 HL-Neg) and low-likelihood infants who did not meet criteria for ASD (n = 145 LL-Neg). Other social communicative skills, like play behaviors and imitation, were also found to be impacted in young autistic children when compared to their non-autistic peers. Understanding early differences in social communication growth before a formal autism diagnosis can provide important insights for early intervention.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Espectro Autista , Transtorno Autístico , Lactente , Humanos , Transtorno Autístico/diagnóstico , Gestos , Transtorno do Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem
2.
Behav Res Methods ; 56(1): 417-432, 2024 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36698000

RESUMO

Occupations are typically characterized in nominal form, a format that limits options for hypothesis testing and data analysis. We drew upon ratings of knowledge, skills, and abilities for 966 occupations listed in the US Department of Labor's Occupational Classification Network (O*NET) database to create an accessible, standardized multidimensional space in which occupations can be quantitatively localized and compared. Principal component analysis revealed that the occupation space comprises three main dimensions that correspond to (1) the required amount of education and training, (2) the degree to which an occupation falls within a science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) discipline versus social sciences and humanities, and (3) whether occupations are more mathematically or health related. Additional occupational spaces reflecting cognitive versus labor-oriented categories were created for finer-grained characterization of dimensions within occupational sets defined by higher or lower required educational preparation. Data-driven groupings of related occupations were obtained with hierarchical cluster analysis (HCA). Proof-of-principle was demonstrated with a real-world dataset (470 participants from the Nathan Kline Institute - Rockland Sample; NKI-RS), whereby verbal and non-verbal abilities-as assessed by standardized testing-were related to the STEM versus social sciences and humanities dimension. Visualization of Latent Components Assessed in O*Net Occupations (VOLCANO) is provided to the research community as a freely accessible tool, along with a Shiny app for users to extract quantitative scores along the relevant dimensions. VOLCANO brings much-needed standardization to unwieldy occupational data. Moreover, it can be used to create new occupational spaces customized to specific research domains.


Assuntos
Ocupações , Humanos , Escolaridade
3.
J Alzheimers Dis ; 93(2): 633-651, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37066909

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Prior work has shown that certain modifiable health, Alzheimer's disease (AD) biomarker, and demographic variables are associated with cognitive performance. However, less is known about the relative importance of these different domains of variables in predicting longitudinal change in cognition. OBJECTIVE: Identify novel relationships between modifiable physical and health variables, AD biomarkers, and slope of cognitive change over two years in a cohort of older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). METHODS: Metrics of cardiometabolic risk, stress, inflammation, neurotrophic/growth factors, and AD pathology were assessed in 123 older adults with MCI at baseline from the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (mean age = 73.9; SD = 7.6; mean education = 16.0; SD = 3.0). Partial least squares regression (PLSR)-a multivariate method which creates components that best predict an outcome-was used to identify whether these physiological variables were important in predicting slope of change in episodic memory or executive function over two years. RESULTS: At two-year follow-up, the two PLSR models predicted, respectively, 20.0% and 19.6% of the variance in change in episodic memory and executive function. Baseline levels of AD biomarkers were important in predicting change in both episodic memory and executive function. Baseline education and neurotrophic/growth factors were important in predicting change in episodic memory, whereas cardiometabolic variables such as blood pressure and cholesterol were important in predicting change in executive function. CONCLUSION: These data-driven analyses highlight the impact of AD biomarkers on cognitive change and further clarify potential domain specific relationships with predictors of cognitive change.


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer , Doenças Cardiovasculares , Disfunção Cognitiva , Humanos , Idoso , Doença de Alzheimer/patologia , Análise dos Mínimos Quadrados , Cognição , Biomarcadores , Doenças Cardiovasculares/complicações , Testes Neuropsicológicos
4.
Dev Cogn Neurosci ; 61: 101240, 2023 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37060675

RESUMO

Decades of research have established that the home language environment, especially quality of caregiver speech, supports language acquisition during infancy. However, the neural mechanisms behind this phenomenon remain under studied. In the current study, we examined associations between the home language environment and structural coherence of white matter tracts in 52 typically developing infants from English speaking homes in a western society. Infants participated in at least one MRI brain scan when they were 3, 6, 12, and/or 24 months old. Home language recordings were collected when infants were 9 and/or 15 months old. General linear regression models indicated that infants who heard the most adult words and participated in the most conversational turns at 9 months of age also had the lowest fractional anisotropy in the left posterior parieto-temporal arcuate fasciculus at 24 months. Similarly, infants who vocalized the most at 9 months also had the lowest fractional anisotropy in the same tract at 6 months of age. This is one of the first studies to report significant associations between caregiver speech collected in the home and white matter structural coherence in the infant brain. The results are in line with prior work showing that protracted white matter development during infancy confers a cognitive advantage.


Assuntos
Substância Branca , Adulto , Humanos , Lactente , Pré-Escolar , Imagem de Tensor de Difusão/métodos , Idioma , Encéfalo , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética
5.
bioRxiv ; 2023 Mar 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36945610

RESUMO

Introduction: Canonical Correlation Analysis (CCA) and Partial Least Squares Correlation (PLS) detect associations between two data matrices based on computing a linear combination between the two matrices (called latent variables; LVs). These LVs maximize correlation (CCA) and covariance (PLS). These different maximization criteria may render one approach more stable and reproducible than the other when working with brain and behavioural data at the population-level. This study compared the LVs which emerged from CCA and PLS analyses of brain-behaviour relationships from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) dataset and examined their stability and reproducibility. Methods: Structural T1-weighted imaging and behavioural data were accessed from the baseline Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development dataset (N > 9000, ages = 9-11 years). The brain matrix consisted of cortical thickness estimates in different cortical regions. The behavioural matrix consisted of 11 subscale scores from the parent-reported Child Behavioral Checklist (CBCL) or 7 cognitive performance measures from the NIH Toolbox. CCA and PLS models were separately applied to the brain-CBCL analysis and brain-cognition analysis. A permutation test was used to assess whether identified LVs were statistically significant. A series of resampling statistical methods were used to assess stability and reproducibility of the LVs. Results: When examining the relationship between cortical thickness and CBCL scores, the first LV was found to be significant across both CCA and PLS models (singular value: CCA = .13, PLS = .39, p < .001). LV1 from the CCA model found that covariation of CBCL scores was linked to covariation of cortical thickness. LV1 from the PLS model identified decreased cortical thickness linked to lower CBCL scores. There was limited evidence of stability or reproducibility of LV1 for both CCA and PLS. When examining the relationship between cortical thickness and cognitive performance, there were 6 significant LVs for both CCA and PLS (p < .01). The first LV showed similar relationships between CCA and PLS and was found to be stable and reproducible (singular value: CCA = .21, PLS = .43, p < .001). Conclusion: CCA and PLS identify different brain-behaviour relationships with limited stability and reproducibility when examining the relationship between cortical thickness and parent-reported behavioural measures. However, both methods identified relatively similar brain-behaviour relationships that were stable and reproducible when examining the relationship between cortical thickness and cognitive performance. The results of the current study suggest that stability and reproducibility of brain-behaviour relationships identified by CCA and PLS are influenced by characteristics of the analyzed sample and the included behavioural measurements when applied to a large pediatric dataset.

6.
Am Ann Deaf ; 167(4): 457-488, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36533478

RESUMO

Teachers of the d/Deaf (TODs) struggle to select appropriate storybooks for elementary-aged Deaf pre-readers who use American Sign Language (Hayes & Shaw, 1994). Hayes and Shaw (1994) created a book selection system for TODs, but their methodology was difficult to evaluate. The purpose of the present research was to create an empirically derived book selection system using a sorting task methodology that has been successfully adapted for creating book selection systems (Schwarz et al., 2015, 2019). In a pilot study, 16 experienced TODs sorted 14 storybooks from published preschool curricula and explained the thinking behind their selections. Using content analysis and a multivariate technique, the authors created a book selection system with a two-tiered glossary and 4-point difficulty scale. They discuss how this system can be used with evidence-based read-aloud interventions compatible with the Framework of Early Literacy (Kuntze & Golos, 2021).


Assuntos
Surdez , Língua de Sinais , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Idoso , Projetos Piloto , Leitura , Alfabetização
7.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37168581

RESUMO

The early emergence of social communication challenges and their impact on language in infants later diagnosed with autism has sparked many early intervention programs that target social communication skills. While research has consistently shown lower scores on social communication assessments in the first year of life, there is limited research at 12-months exploring associations between different dimensions of social communication and later language. Understanding associations between early social communication skills and language would enhance our ability to choose high priority intervention goals that will impact downstream language skills. The current study used a standardized assessment to profile social communication skills across 516 infants with a high (HL) or low likelihood (LL-Neg) for autism (84% White, 60% Male), based on the presence of a sibling with autism in the family. The primary aim of the study was to profile social communication skill development in the second year of life and to evaluate associations between social communication skills and later language. HL infants who met criteria for autism (HL-ASD, N = 81) demonstrated widespread reductions in social communication skills at 12-months compared to HL infants who did not meet criteria for autism (HL-Neg, N = 277) and LL-Neg (N = 158) infants. Across all infants in the study, those with better social communication skills at 12-months had better language at 24-months. However, within group analyses indicated that infants who met criteria for autism did not show this developmental coupling until 24-months-of-age at which point social communication was positively associated with downstream language skills. The cascading pattern of reduced social communication skills as well as overall significant positive associations with later language provide further evidence for the need to support developing social communication skills prior to formal autism diagnosis, a goal that could possibly be reached through pre-emptive interventions.

8.
Data Brief ; 39: 107573, 2021 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34877370

RESUMO

We provide functional connectivity matrices generated during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during different tasks of cognitive control in healthy aging adults. These data can be used to replicate the primary results from the related manuscript: Reconfiguration and dedifferentiation of functional networks during cognitive control across the adult lifespan (Rieck et al., 2021). One-hundred-forty-four participants (ages 20-86) were scanned on a Siemens 3T MRI scanner while they were completing tasks to measure functional activity during inhibition, initiation, shifting, and working memory. Estimates of functional connectivity (quantified with timeseries correlations) between different brain regions were computed using three different brain atlases: Schaefer 100 parcel 17 network atlas (Schaefer et al., 2018; Yeo et al., 2011), Power 229 node 10 network atlas (Power et al., 2011), and Schaefer 200 parcel 17 network atlas (Schaefer et al., 2018; Yeo et al., 2011). The resulting functional connectivity correlation matrices are provided as text files with this article. Cov-STATIS (Abdi et al., 2012; a multi-table multivariate statistical technique; https://github.com/HerveAbdi/DistatisR) was used to examine similarity between functional connectivity during the different domains of cognitive control. The effect of aging on these functional connectivity patterns was also examined by computing measures of "task differentiation" and "network segregation." This dataset also provides supplemental analyses from the related manuscript (Rieck et al., 2021) to replicate the primary age findings with additional brain atlases. Cognitive neuroscience researchers can benefit from these data by further investigating the age effects on functional connectivity during tasks of cognitive control, in addition to examining the impact of different brain atlases on functional connectivity estimates. These data can also be used for the development of other multi-table and network-based statistical methods in functional neuroimaging.

9.
Neurobiol Aging ; 106: 80-94, 2021 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34256190

RESUMO

Healthy aging is accompanied by reduced cognitive control and widespread alterations in the underlying brain networks; but the extent to which large-scale functional networks in older age show reduced specificity across different domains of cognitive control is unclear. Here we use cov-STATIS (a multi-table multivariate technique) to examine similarity of functional connectivity during different domains of cognitive control-inhibition, initiation, shifting, and working memory-across the adult lifespan. We report two major findings: (1) Functional connectivity patterns during initiation, inhibition, and shifting were more similar in older ages, particularly for control and default networks, a pattern consistent with dedifferentiation of the neural correlates associated with cognitive control; and (2) Networks exhibited age-related reconfiguration such that frontal, default, and dorsal attention networks were more integrated whereas sub-networks of somato-motor system were more segregated in older age. Together these findings offer new evidence for dedifferentiation and reconfiguration of functional connectivity underlying different aspects of cognitive control in normal aging.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Desdiferenciação Celular/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Envelhecimento Saudável/fisiologia , Longevidade/fisiologia , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Feminino , Envelhecimento Saudável/psicologia , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Rede Nervosa/diagnóstico por imagem , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
10.
Mem Cognit ; 49(2): 265-275, 2021 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33051816

RESUMO

Influential research has focused on identifying the common neural and behavioural substrates underlying episodic memory (the re-experiencing of specific details from past experiences) and spatial cognition, with some theories proposing that these are supported by the same mechanisms. However, the similarities and differences between these two forms of memory in humans require further specification. We used an individual-differences approach based on self-reported survey data collected in a large online study (n = 7,487), focusing on autobiographical episodic memory and spatial navigation and their relationship to object and spatial imagery abilities. Multivariate analyses replicated prior findings that autobiographical episodic memory abilities dissociated from spatial navigational abilities. Considering imagery, episodic autobiographical memory overlapped with imagery of objects, whereas spatial navigation overlapped with a tendency to focus on spatial schematics and manipulation. These results suggest that trait episodic autobiographical memory and spatial navigation correspond to distinct mental processes.


Assuntos
Memória Episódica , Navegação Espacial , Cognição , Humanos , Individualidade , Rememoração Mental
11.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 15901, 2020 09 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32985550

RESUMO

The dynamic structure of proteins is essential for their functions and may include large conformational transitions which can be studied by molecular dynamics (MD) simulations. However, details of these transitions are difficult to automatically track. To facilitate their analysis, we developed two scores of correlation between sidechain dihedral angles. The CIRCULAR and OMES scores are computed from, respectively, dihedral angle values and rotamer distributions. As a case study, we applied our methods to an activation-like transition of the chemokine receptor CXCR4, observed during accelerated MD simulations. The principal component analysis of the correlation matrices was consistent with the networking structure of the top ranking pairs. Both scores identify a set of residues whose "collaborative" sidechain rotamerization immediately preceded or accompanied the conformational transition of CXCR4. Detailed analysis of the sequential order of these rotamerizations suggests that an allosteric mechanism, involving the outward motion of an asparagine residue in transmembrane helix 3, might be a prerequisite to the large scale conformational transition of CXCR4. This case study provides the proof-of-concept that the correlation methods developed here are valuable exploratory techniques to help decipher complex reactional pathways.


Assuntos
Simulação de Dinâmica Molecular , Conformação Proteica , Proteínas/química
12.
Cognition ; 202: 104309, 2020 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32388006

RESUMO

To bridge the gap between naturalistic and laboratory assessments of episodic memory, we designed time- and content-matched real-world and virtualized versions of the same tour event. In younger and older adults, we investigated objective and subjective aspects of recollection for event features using a verbal true/false test common to both event conditions. Using a data-driven multivariate analysis blind to the age groups and event conditions, we found that discriminating altered from true details accounted for the largest amount of variance in objective retrieval patterns. There was an advantage for real-world over laboratory encoding on this dimension for both age groups. Similarly, real-world encoding elicited higher scores on a dimension defined by subjective recollection. However, real-world (but not laboratory) encoding decoupled objective and subjective memory in older adults, who reported similar rates of subjective recollection as younger adults despite exhibiting significantly poorer discrimination accuracy. These results demonstrate robust and specific ways in which the accuracy and subjective quality of memory differ for matched naturalistic and laboratory episodes. Furthermore, these results suggest that naturalistic and laboratory encoding conditions produce qualitatively different patterns of episodic memory decline in older age.


Assuntos
Laboratórios , Memória Episódica , Idoso , Envelhecimento , Humanos , Rememoração Mental
13.
Ear Hear ; 41(3): 508-520, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31592903

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Efficient multisensory speech detection is critical for children who must quickly detect/encode a rapid stream of speech to participate in conversations and have access to the audiovisual cues that underpin speech and language development, yet multisensory speech detection remains understudied in children with hearing loss (CHL). This research assessed detection, along with vigilant/goal-directed attention, for multisensory versus unisensory speech in CHL versus children with normal hearing (CNH). DESIGN: Participants were 60 CHL who used hearing aids and communicated successfully aurally/orally and 60 age-matched CNH. Simple response times determined how quickly children could detect a preidentified easy-to-hear stimulus (70 dB SPL, utterance "buh" presented in auditory only [A], visual only [V], or audiovisual [AV] modes). The V mode formed two facial conditions: static versus dynamic face. Faster detection for multisensory (AV) than unisensory (A or V) input indicates multisensory facilitation. We assessed mean responses and faster versus slower responses (defined by first versus third quartiles of response-time distributions), which were respectively conceptualized as: faster responses (first quartile) reflect efficient detection with efficient vigilant/goal-directed attention and slower responses (third quartile) reflect less efficient detection associated with attentional lapses. Finally, we studied associations between these results and personal characteristics of CHL. RESULTS: Unisensory A versus V modes: Both groups showed better detection and attention for A than V input. The A input more readily captured children's attention and minimized attentional lapses, which supports A-bound processing even by CHL who were processing low fidelity A input. CNH and CHL did not differ in ability to detect A input at conversational speech level. Multisensory AV versus A modes: Both groups showed better detection and attention for AV than A input. The advantage for AV input was facial effect (both static and dynamic faces), a pattern suggesting that communication is a social interaction that is more than just words. Attention did not differ between groups; detection was faster in CHL than CNH for AV input, but not for A input. Associations between personal characteristics/degree of hearing loss of CHL and results: CHL with greatest deficits in detection of V input had poorest word recognition skills and CHL with greatest reduction of attentional lapses from AV input had poorest vocabulary skills. Both outcomes are consistent with the idea that CHL who are processing low fidelity A input depend disproportionately on V and AV input to learn to identify words and associate them with concepts. As CHL aged, attention to V input improved. Degree of HL did not influence results. CONCLUSIONS: Understanding speech-a daily challenge for CHL-is a complex task that demands efficient detection of and attention to AV speech cues. Our results support the clinical importance of multisensory approaches to understand and advance spoken communication by CHL.


Assuntos
Surdez , Perda Auditiva , Percepção da Fala , Idoso , Criança , Humanos , Tempo de Reação , Fala , Percepção Visual
14.
Depress Anxiety ; 36(8): 753-765, 2019 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31066992

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Prenatal maternal depression (PMD) and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) antidepressants are associated with increased developmental risk in infants. Reports suggest that PMD is associated with hyperconnectivity of the insula and the amygdala, while SSRI exposure is associated with hyperconnectivity of the auditory network in the infant brain. However, associations between functional brain organization and PMD and/or SSRI exposure are not well understood. METHODS: We examined the relation between PMD or SSRI exposure and neonatal brain functional organization. Infants of control (n = 17), depressed SSRI-treated (n = 20) and depressed-only (HAM-D ≥ 8) (n = 16) women, underwent resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging at postnatal Day 6. At 6 months, temperament was assessed using Infant Behavioral Questionnaire (IBQ). We applied GTA and partial least square regression (PLSR) to the resting-state time series to assess group differences in modularity, and connector and provincial hubs. RESULTS: Modularity was similar across all groups. The depressed-only group showed higher connector hub values in the left anterior cingulate, insula, and caudate as well as higher provincial hub values in the amygdala compared to the control group. The SSRI group showed higher provincial hub values in Heschl's gyrus relative to the depressed-only group. PLSR showed that newborns' hub values predicted 10% of the variability in infant temperament at 6 months, suggesting different developmental patterns between groups. CONCLUSIONS: Prenatal exposures to maternal depression and SSRIs have differential impacts on neonatal functional brain organization. Hub values at 6 days predict variance in temperament between infant groups at 6 months of age.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Transtorno Depressivo/tratamento farmacológico , Mães/psicologia , Complicações na Gravidez/fisiopatologia , Efeitos Tardios da Exposição Pré-Natal/fisiopatologia , Inibidores Seletivos de Recaptação de Serotonina/uso terapêutico , Adulto , Antidepressivos/uso terapêutico , Encéfalo/efeitos dos fármacos , Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Desenvolvimento Infantil/efeitos dos fármacos , Transtorno Depressivo/fisiopatologia , Transtorno Depressivo/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Vias Neurais/diagnóstico por imagem , Vias Neurais/efeitos dos fármacos , Gravidez , Complicações na Gravidez/tratamento farmacológico , Complicações na Gravidez/psicologia , Temperamento/efeitos dos fármacos
15.
PLoS One ; 14(3): e0211463, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30865639

RESUMO

We propose a new sparsification method for the singular value decomposition-called the constrained singular value decomposition (CSVD)-that can incorporate multiple constraints such as sparsification and orthogonality for the left and right singular vectors. The CSVD can combine different constraints because it implements each constraint as a projection onto a convex set, and because it integrates these constraints as projections onto the intersection of multiple convex sets. We show that, with appropriate sparsification constants, the algorithm is guaranteed to converge to a stable point. We also propose and analyze the convergence of an efficient algorithm for the specific case of the projection onto the balls defined by the norms L1 and L2. We illustrate the CSVD and compare it to the standard singular value decomposition and to a non-orthogonal related sparsification method with: 1) a simulated example, 2) a small set of face images (corresponding to a configuration with a number of variables much larger than the number of observations), and 3) a psychometric application with a large number of observations and a small number of variables. The companion R-package, csvd, that implements the algorithms described in this paper, along with reproducible examples, are available for download from https://github.com/vguillemot/csvd.


Assuntos
Algoritmos , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Simulação por Computador , Bases de Dados Factuais/estatística & dados numéricos , Face/anatomia & histologia , Feminino , Humanos , Imaginação , Masculino , Modelos Estatísticos , Análise Multivariada , Reconhecimento Automatizado de Padrão/estatística & dados numéricos , Análise de Componente Principal , Psicometria/estatística & dados numéricos
16.
J Food Sci ; 84(3): 629-639, 2019 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30730566

RESUMO

We present a Descriptive Analysis (DA) of a large representative sample (24 whiskeys) of two legally distinct types of American whiskeys: bourbon and rye whiskey (respectively distilled from a fermented "mashbill" of at least 51% corn or rye). We wanted to determine whether a trained panel could find sensory differences between these two products. We used standard DA: 11 judges were trained for 10 hours to develop a lexicon of 24 flavor, taste, and mouthfeel descriptors for the 24 whiskey samples (15 bourbons and 9 ryes). Then, subjects rated each whiskey sample on each attribute, using unstructured line scales, in standard good sensory-evaluation conditions, and in triplicate. Results were analyzed using MANOVA, Barycentric Discriminant Analysis, and Hierarchical Cluster Analysis. Overall, while 10 descriptors significantly differentiated the whiskeys, no attribute or combination of attributes was predicted by mashbill: The judges did not find differences in sensory character between bourbon and rye whiskeys as categories. However, significant differences could be attributed to the producer of the whiskey and to age at bottling. These results are important because American whiskey has recently become staggeringly popular, and because there is a consistent belief that bourbon and rye whiskeys-as categories-have distinct sensory characteristics. PRACTICAL APPLICATION: This research contradicts popular and expert beliefs about the distinction between the popular and important American rye and bourbon whiskeys. A comprehensive sensory DA study of 24 American whiskeys with different mashbills (9 ryes and 15 bourbons) shows that-while each whiskey is individually distinct on a number of sensory dimensions-sensory differences between rye and bourbon whiskeys as product categories cannot be predicted by mashbill. For producers, consumers, and researchers, this research points to the need for new theories on the origins of flavors in whiskey, as it is now clear that the grain content alone cannot predict sensory qualities.


Assuntos
Bebidas Alcoólicas/análise , Adulto , Análise Discriminante , Feminino , Aromatizantes , Humanos , Masculino , Paladar
17.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 61(12): 3095-3112, 2018 12 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30515515

RESUMO

Purpose: Successful speech processing depends on our ability to detect and integrate multisensory cues, yet there is minimal research on multisensory speech detection and integration by children. To address this need, we studied the development of speech detection for auditory (A), visual (V), and audiovisual (AV) input. Method: Participants were 115 typically developing children clustered into age groups between 4 and 14 years. Speech detection (quantified by response times [RTs]) was determined for 1 stimulus, /buh/, presented in A, V, and AV modes (articulating vs. static facial conditions). Performance was analyzed not only in terms of traditional mean RTs but also in terms of the faster versus slower RTs (defined by the 1st vs. 3rd quartiles of RT distributions). These time regions were conceptualized respectively as reflecting optimal detection with efficient focused attention versus less optimal detection with inefficient focused attention due to attentional lapses. Results: Mean RTs indicated better detection (a) of multisensory AV speech than A speech only in 4- to 5-year-olds and (b) of A and AV inputs than V input in all age groups. The faster RTs revealed that AV input did not improve detection in any group. The slower RTs indicated that (a) the processing of silent V input was significantly faster for the articulating than static face and (b) AV speech or facial input significantly minimized attentional lapses in all groups except 6- to 7-year-olds (a peaked U-shaped curve). Apparently, the AV benefit observed for mean performance in 4- to 5-year-olds arose from effects of attention. Conclusions: The faster RTs indicated that AV input did not enhance detection in any group, but the slower RTs indicated that AV speech and dynamic V speech (mouthing) significantly minimized attentional lapses and thus did influence performance. Overall, A and AV inputs were detected consistently faster than V input; this result endorsed stimulus-bound auditory processing by these children.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Adolescente , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Tempo de Reação
18.
Neuroscience ; 390: 79-87, 2018 10 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30134205

RESUMO

Blind individuals display superior sensory abilities in other modalities, yet results remain contradictory regarding their performance on olfactory tasks. Using complex ecological olfactory tasks, we evaluated the impact of blindness on olfactory performance. We tested 12 early-blind individuals (M = 49, SD = 13.09) and 12 sighted controls (M = 49, SD = 14.31) who were all blindfolded. Based solely on the wine odors, participants evaluated 24 pairs of wine and determined if both samples belonged to the same category (red wine, white wine, or rosé wine) or not (odor categorization), and if so, whether they were identical or not (odor differentiation). Then, they had to classify 15 different wines (5 red, 5 white and 5 rosé) into red, white, and rosé wines (odor classification). Blind individuals (d': M = 1.3, SD = 1.2) presented lower scores compared to sighted controls (M = 2.2 SD = 0.8; p < .05) in the odor categorization task, but no group difference was observed for the other tasks. For all participants, red wine odors were the easiest to classify (1.8 ±â€¯1.0), followed by white wine odors (0.5 ±â€¯0.6) and finally rosé wine odors (blind and sighted; F[2; 44] = 11.9, p < .001). In summary, early-blind individuals had a harder time to categorize wine odors. This could be explained by a different construction of internal reference categories for wine in early-blind individuals. Finally, this research is in line with the notion of the absence of higher olfactory sensitivity in blind individuals.


Assuntos
Cegueira/psicologia , Percepção Olfatória , Vinho , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Odorantes , Detecção de Sinal Psicológico , Olfato , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
19.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 14(6): e1006209, 2018 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29912865

RESUMO

Chemokines and their receptors (members of the GPCR super-family) are involved in a wide variety of physiological processes and diseases; thus, understanding the specificity of the chemokine receptor family could help develop new receptor specific drugs. Here, we explore the evolutionary mechanisms that led to the emergence of the chemokine receptors. Based on GPCR hierarchical classification, we analyzed nested GPCR sets with an eigen decomposition approach of the sequence covariation matrix and determined three key residues whose mutation was crucial for the emergence of the chemokine receptors and their subsequent divergence into homeostatic and inflammatory receptors. These residues are part of the allosteric sodium binding site. Their structural and functional roles were investigated by molecular dynamics simulations of CXCR4 and CCR5 as prototypes of homeostatic and inflammatory chemokine receptors, respectively. This study indicates that the three mutations crucial for the evolution of the chemokine receptors dramatically altered the sodium binding mode. In CXCR4, the sodium ion is tightly bound by four protein atoms and one water molecule. In CCR5, the sodium ion is mobile within the binding pocket and moves between different sites involving from one to three protein atoms and two to five water molecules. Analysis of chemokine receptor evolution reveals that a highly constrained sodium binding site characterized most ancient receptors, and that the constraints were subsequently loosened during the divergence of this receptor family. We discuss the implications of these findings for the evolution of the chemokine receptor functions and mechanisms of action.


Assuntos
Receptores CCR5/genética , Receptores CXCR4/genética , Sódio/metabolismo , Sítio Alostérico , Sequência de Aminoácidos/genética , Sítios de Ligação/genética , Sítios de Ligação/fisiologia , Evolução Biológica , Quimiocinas/genética , Quimiocinas/metabolismo , Simulação por Computador , Evolução Molecular , Humanos , Simulação de Dinâmica Molecular , Mutação/genética , Filogenia , Análise de Componente Principal/métodos , Ligação Proteica/genética , Conformação Proteica , Receptores CCR5/fisiologia , Receptores CXCR4/fisiologia , Receptores de Quimiocinas/genética , Receptores de Quimiocinas/fisiologia , Transdução de Sinais
20.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 39(7): 2764-2776, 2018 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29575246

RESUMO

The functional neuroimaging literature has become increasingly complex and thus difficult to navigate. This complexity arises from the rate at which new studies are published and from the terminology that varies widely from study-to-study and even more so from discipline-to-discipline. One way to investigate and manage this problem is to build a "semantic space" that maps the different vocabulary used in functional neuroimaging literature. Such a semantic space will also help identify the primary research domains of neuroimaging and their most commonly reported brain regions. In this work, we analyzed the multivariate semantic structure of abstracts in Neurosynth and found that there are six primary domains of the functional neuroimaging literature, each with their own preferred reported brain regions. Our analyses also highlight possible semantic sources of reported brain regions within and across domains because some research topics (e.g., memory disorders, substance use disorder) use heterogeneous terminology. Furthermore, we highlight the growth and decline of the primary domains over time. Finally, we note that our techniques and results form the basis of a "recommendation engine" that could help readers better navigate the neuroimaging literature.


Assuntos
Bibliometria , Encéfalo , Mineração de Dados , Neuroimagem Funcional , Terminologia como Assunto , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Humanos , Semântica
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...